ВУЗ: Не указан
Категория: Не указан
Дисциплина: Не указана
Добавлен: 06.12.2020
Просмотров: 1072
Скачиваний: 3
68
Lecture 18: Myths about Magnets, Microwaves, Cell Phones
Flamm, “Magnet Therapy.”
Mesmer,
Mesmerism.
1.
What effects do magnets have on the human body?
2.
Why do you think magnets are such a common target for
dubious remedies?
Questions to Consider
Suggested Reading
69
All about Hypnosis
Lecture 19
Have you ever been daydreaming while driving in the car and arrived
at your location without remembering how you got there? ... Much
of what happens in the brain while you’re daydreaming—not paying
attention to external stimuli, although still being able to process
them enough to drive to your destination while imagining being
somewhere else or in another situation—is very similar to what happens
during hypnosis.
L
ike most people, you probably know hypnosis from what you’ve seen
on stage or on television. An ordinary person is put into a trance-like
state and then starts walking around clucking like a chicken on cue
because they were hypnotized to do so. What is hypnosis really? The
¿
rst
myth about hypnosis I’d like to put to rest is that it’s a trance-like state. In
fact, it isn’t an altered state of consciousness or unique state of consciousness.
It’s actually a state of heightened alertness.
How do you hypnotize someone? It really just has to do with how you interact
with the person. For example, the process might involve making someone
more suggestible and encouraging them to relax, encouraging them to focus
on the person who’s doing the hypnotizing, giving them small suggestions
to reinforce their attention, and encouraging visualization. Stage hypnosis
is different from medical hypnosis; the latter is the topic for the rest of
this lecture.
There is a very serious neuroscience surrounding what happens in the
brain when people are being hypnotized. These effects fall under 4 broad
categories: increased suggestibility; heightened imagination; a lack of
attention to sensory information, even a sense of detachment from one’s self
or environment; and a decrease in executive function, which is the highest
order of thinking.
Can hypnosis be used for bene
¿
cial medical or psychological effects? Let’s
turn
¿
rst to memory. Can we hypnotize people who are having trouble
70
Lecture 19:
All about Hypnosis
remembering details and get them to recall an event in vivid detail, as is
often portrayed on television? It turns out that we cannot. In fact, if anything,
the opposite is true. Hypnosis is a condition in which a person is in a high
state of imagination and can be highly suggestible. Therefore, patients are
likely to make up details at the slightest suggestion.
But there are indeed some legitimate uses of hypnosis. Hypnoanesthesia is
the use of hypnosis prior to a surgical or medical procedure to reduce the
need for sedating medication. This usually involves self-hypnosis: The
person uses techniques like meditation and imagery. This self-hypnosis has
been shown to minimize, but not eliminate, the need for sedation. Local
anesthesia is often still used. Very
closely related to hypnoanesthesia is
hypnoanalgesia. Reviews of evidence
indicate that hypnoanalgesia is in fact
useful for decreasing chronic pain.
Cognitive hypnotherapy uses hypnosis
to treat depression, sleep disorders,
chronic pain, and post-traumatic
stress disorder. The techniques
used involve meditation, cognitive therapy, and self-hypnosis. Cognitive
therapy is a separate part of behavioral treatment or talk therapy for all of
these conditions, and it has independently been shown to be effective. When
hypnosis, mainly involving meditation, is combined with cognitive therapy,
it’s shown to have signi
¿
cant advantages.
I’ve mentioned several times the notion of meditation as a form of self-
hypnosis. Meditation is not hypnosis exactly, but it is closely related. It is
a self-induced state of relaxation. This relaxation may also involve active
thinking, self-re
À
ection, or an attempt to achieve a state of what is called
mental silence—essentially thinking about nothing.
There are two basic types of meditation and various uses to which they are
put. One is called concentration meditation. In concentration meditation,
practitioners focus on an object or idea as an anchor to focus their thoughts.
In mindfulness meditation, the awareness is not focused on one thing but is
Hypnoanesthesia is the
use of hypnosis prior
to a surgical or medical
procedure to reduce the need
for sedating medication.
71
distributed as broadly as possible. Their awareness is diffused to everything
in the environment, which puts them in a state similar to mental silence.
There is evidence to support medical uses for these types of meditation
for pain, blood pressure, stress management, and muscle relaxation. So
far, though, there is no evidence to support meditation for psychological
conditions like attention de
¿
cit and hyperactivity disorder or anxiety.
Ŷ
Jamieson,
Hypnosis and Conscious States
.
Lynn and Kirsch,
Essentials of Clinical Hypnosis.
1.
Is hypnotism real? Do scientists know what is happening in the brain
when someone is “hypnotized”?
2.
Does hypnosis have legitimate clinical uses?
Suggested Reading
Questions to Consider
72
Lecture 20: Myths about Coma and Consciousness
Myths about Coma and Consciousness
Lecture 20
There is a famous case of a man named Jean-Dominique Bauby, a
French journalist, who had a stroke in 1995 at the age of 43. It left
him locked in. He could only blink his left eye. However, he dictated
the entire book
The Diving Bell and the Butter
À
y
one letter at a time
by blinking.
T
V and movies are full of stories in which someone is in a coma and
then at some point, maybe even after years, they wake up—largely
neurologically intact. The media also loves stories about people
awaking from a coma. This all contributes to a lot of confusion about what
coma actually is.
Coma is a disorder of consciousness or wakefulness. What are the causes
of coma? One is trauma. Damage to enough neurons can impair the brain’s
ability to generate enough function to be awake. Another cause is diffuse
anoxic/ischemic injury, in which something interrupts the blood
À
ow to parts
of or all of the brain. This causes enough damage to parts of the brain that
you cannot generate consciousness.
A completely different phenomenon that can impair consciousness is a
seizure. A seizure is typically a synchronized, abnormal electrical function
where brain cells start to
¿
re in unison. This can happen in one part of the
brain or can spread throughout the brain. If a seizure occurs in enough of the
brain, it can cause a person to be unconscious. Prolonged seizures can even
make a person appear to be in a prolonged coma.
One myth about coma that I want to dispel is that coma is a speci
¿
c brain
state—like a switch that is either on or off. In fact, coma is a continuum.
Diagnosing coma offers a lot of challenges to the neurologist. The primary
problem is that a lot of the neurological exam is based upon a person being
awake and being able to follow complex instructions and answer complex
questions. That enables us to probe and query the different functions of the
different parts of the brain. Another challenge is that consciousness may be