Файл: Medical_Myths_Lies_and_Half-Truths_guidebook.pdf

ВУЗ: Не указан

Категория: Не указан

Дисциплина: Не указана

Добавлен: 06.12.2020

Просмотров: 1099

Скачиваний: 3

ВНИМАНИЕ! Если данный файл нарушает Ваши авторские права, то обязательно сообщите нам.
background image

23

Can we in

À

 uence the ecosystem of bacteria in our body with what we eat? 

That’s the basic concept of probiotics. However, that concept is a bit 

À

 awed. 

The primary conceptual problem here is that the ecosystem is easily altered. 
In fact, it’s not easy to alter it at all. It’s very dif

¿

 cult for a new bacterial 

species to work its way into that ecosystem. 

Concepts aside, what does the evidence actually show? Do probiotic products 
work for any speci

¿

 c indication? There are some indications for which a 

mild bene

¿

 t has been shown for some probiotic products. However, that is 

only the case when treatment is given very early and consists of probiotics 
with high colony counts that contain several species. 

Let’s talk about some speci

¿

 c uses. One use is preventing or treating 

diarrhea resulting from antibiotics. It turns out if you have an infection 

with 

C. dif

¿

 cile

, the evidence 

shows that probiotics are of no 
bene

¿

 t.  What  about  irritable 

bowel syndrome, a very common 
disorder? There is weak evidence 
of a mild bene

¿

 t, but the best that 

researchers could say at this time 
is that more research is needed. 
Probiotics have also been tested in 

allergies. A 2008 systematic review of the evidence for a speci

¿

 c type of 

allergy called atopic dermatitis found only mixed results. Do probiotics 
work for 

H. pylori

? There’s preliminary evidence for a mild bene

¿

 t, not 

by itself, but as what we call adjunctive therapy. If you’re taking the other 
treatments that have been shown to be effective for 

H. pylori

 and add 

probiotics, you may have a mild advantage.

Most of the probiotic market is actually for routine use. Here I think the 
evidence is pretty clear: If you’re a healthy individual with your normal 
bacterial ecosystem, then eating speci

¿

 c types of live bacteria simply doesn’t 

have any bene

¿

 t. It also should be noted that we are constantly exposed to 

bacteria from our environment. Adding a few extra bacteria in a speci

¿

 c 

yogurt doesn’t really add much to our environmental exposure to bacteria. 
The bottom line is there is no evidence for routine use. While probiotics and 

People tend to think of bacteria 
as germs—things that cause 
disease—when that is mostly 
not true. 


background image

24

Lecture 6: Probiotics and Our Bacterial Friends

prebiotics are more hype than help currently, there is evidence that there may 
be some potential symptomatic bene

¿

 t for speci

¿

 c medical conditions. We 

may be able to affect human health with the probiotic approach, but we’re 
not there yet. 

Ŷ

Crislip, “Probiotics.” 

Floch and Kim, 

Probiotics.

1. 

What is the role of friendly bacteria in human health?

2. 

Are there any proven uses for probiotics?

    Suggested Reading

    Questions to Consider


background image

25

Sugar and Hyperactivity

Lecture 7

In fact—and this seems somewhat counterintuitive—because caffeine is 
a stimulant, it may improve attention and stimulate the frontal lobes to 
function a little bit better. It may, paradoxically, decrease hyperactivity 
or improve attention in children.

E

very parent knows that kids have a ton of energy, and hyperactivity 
may just be a natural part of being young. But, in some children, it can 
actually be a disorder, a disability that hampers school performance 

and makes home life challenging. The search for a cause and a cure for 
excessive hyperactivity in kids has led down many blind alleys. It has led to 
an industry of self-help books leaving parents with a tremendous amount of 
information, including a lot of misinformation.

One of the biggest hypotheses—and perhaps the biggest myth dealing with 
childhood behavior—is the food hypothesis: Children behave the way they 
do because of the food that they eat. This notion that there’s a link between 
food and hyperactivity goes back to the 1920s and has been controversial ever 
since. It was mostly popularized in the 1970s by Benjamin Feingold, who 
created the Feingold diet. This is a diet that removes many things, including 
food coloring, from children’s diets to eliminate or reduce hyperactivity. 

A comprehensive review of the evidence performed in the 1980s showed 
that there is no link between additives and food, and hyperactivity or 
behavioral changes. But a recent study showed a weak correlation between 
food coloring and parents noticing an increase in their child’s activity. It is 
possible that there is a mild effect in a small subset, about 5%, of children. 

There are also those who think that sugar is the culprit. Despite this common 
belief with its obvious source in casual observations that most parents would 
make, there is no link. There is no evidence to support a link between eating 
lots of sugar and any behavioral change. 


background image

26

Lecture 7: Sugar and Hyperactivity

What about allergies? I’ve had parents tell me that they think that their child 
has a food allergy, and that the allergic reaction is behavioral changes. Real 
allergies cause skin rashes, breathing problems, sleeping dif

¿

 culty,  and 

generally feeling under the weather. Allergies do not cause hyperactivity or 
other behavioral changes. 

Attention de

¿

 cit hyperactivity disorder, or ADHD, is diagnosed in children 

who are far enough to the hyperactive end of the spectrum that it begins 
to impair their ability to function at home, at school, and in other 
situations. ADHD is 
best understood as a 
relative de

¿

 ciency  of 

executive function in 
the brain. Executive 
function comes from 
our frontal lobes, 
which give us the 
ability to look at the 
big picture, to think 
about the consequences 
of our actions. 

How do we diagnose 
ADHD? There’s no 
blood test. There’s no MRI scan. There’s no de

¿

 nitive objective biological 

test to say who has ADHD and who doesn’t. With spectrums, there’s no 
absolute objective place to draw the line. To meet the criteria for that 
diagnosis, children or adults need to have at least 6 speci

¿

 c  symptoms. 

The symptoms need to be of at least 6 month’s duration and present in 2 or 
more settings. 

How common is ADHD? By the strict diagnostic criteria, about 3% to 8% 
of children can be diagnosed with ADHD. About 50% of them will continue 
to meet those criteria into adulthood. It is a bit of a myth that all children 
with ADHD will outgrow their symptoms; only 50% do. A claim that 
comes up frequently is that ADHD is overdiagnosed. I think it is important 
that we do due diligence to make sure that we’re using our diagnostic 

No link has been shown between eating sugar and 
behavioral change.

© BananaStock/Thinkstock.


background image

27

criteria appropriately. This has been speci

¿

 cally studied. If ADHD were 

overdiagnosed, you would expect that the false positive rate would exceed 
the false negative rate. But in speci

¿

 c studies, they 

¿

 nd that there’s no 

difference. ADHD is actually not overdiagnosed, despite the very popular 
belief that it is.

Is ADHD overtreated? There is an increase in the use of medication over 
recent years, but studies have shown that this increase is mainly because 
previously underserved populations are now being treated. A higher 
percentage of people with ADHD are being treated. 

While ADHD is a genuine disorder, it is also part of a spectrum of typical 
childhood behavior and is highly treatable. We have very effective methods 
of improving behavior and outcomes in children. There’s also a lot that 
we’ve learned about what to do for your typical child who has the typical 
range of hyperactivity—the kind of thing that all parents deal with. 

There are a great number of myths out there about what triggers hyperactivity. 
A lot of it revolves around food—sugar, caffeine, and food additives. These 
serve as a distraction from the truth, and this is one of the big downsides of 
myths. Misinformation is often more harmful than just ignorance. Parents 
focus their efforts on highly restrictive diets that are very dif

¿

 cult and that 

may cause more problems for their child. They are better off focusing on 
basic parenting skills, forming a working relationship with their children, 
and focusing on the behavioral modi

¿

 cation techniques that have been 

effective for decades. 

Ŷ

Hallowell and Ratey, 

Driven to Distraction

.

MedlinePlus, “Hyperactivity and Sugar.” 

    Suggested Reading